Parkinsonism and Related Disorders 14 (2008) 224–228
Striatal FDOPA uptake and cognition in advanced non-demented
Parkinson’s disease: A clinical and FDOPA-PET study
Marije van Beilena,b,Ã, Axel T. Portmana, Henk A.L. Kiersc, Ralph P. Maguirea,b,
Valtteri Kaasinena, Marthe Koninga, Jan Pruimb,d, Klaus L. Leendersa,b
aDepartment of Neurology, University Medical Center Groningen, University of Groningen, P.O. Box 30001, 9700 RB Groningen, The Netherlands
bSchool for Behavioral and Cognitive Neurosciences (BCN), University Medical Center, Groningen, The Netherlands
cHeymans Institute, University of Groningen, The Netherlands
dPET Center, University Medical Center Groningen, University of Groningen, The Netherlands
Received 30 January 2007; received in revised form 18 July 2007; accepted 14 August 2007
This study sought to determine the nature of the relationship between cognition and striatal dopaminergic functioning in 28 patients
with advanced Parkinson’s disease (PD) using fluorodopa Positron emission tomography (FDOPA-PET) and neuropsychological testscores. Mental flexibility was related to putamen activity while mental organization (executive memory and fluency) was related tocaudate FDOPA uptake. Interestingly, the caudate may be more important in the mental components of executive functioning, while theputamen may be more important in the motor components of executive functioning. r 2007 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Keywords: Parkinson; Cognition; Striatum; FDOPA-PET
components of memory performance are disturbed, while thenon-executive storage components are relatively intact.
Idiopathic Parkinson’s disease (PD) is a neurodegenera-
A possible explanation for the cognitive deficits can be
tive disorder of unknown etiology, characterized by
found in the striatal connections with the frontal lobes .
progressive loss of dopaminergic neurons in the nigros-
Positron emission tomography (PET), using the radiotracer
triatal pathway PD is clinically featured by its motor
6-L (18F)-fluorodopa (FDOPA), provides a means of
symptoms consisting of an insidious (asymmetric) onset of
quantifying the loss of striatal dopaminergic terminal
bradykinesia, rigidity and (rest) tremor .
function in vivo in PD. Specific striatal FDOPA uptake
However, PD is also often accompanied by cognitive
reflects nigrostriatal dopamine storage capacity and enzy-
impairments. Cognitive deficits are found in several domains
matic decarboxylase activity in surviving neurons .
including Memory and the Executive functions, i.e. deficient
According to the current model of basal ganglia organiza-
cognitive switching, impaired concept formation and res-
tion, dopaminergic connections between the caudate nucleus
ponse-inhibition, impaired planning abilities and impaired
and frontal areas are more strongly related to cognition and
fluency performance Deficits in the Executive functions
less strongly related to motor function. Putaminal FDOPA
can negatively influence other cognitive domains too. Most
uptake has been related to motor function in both early and
importantly, memory profiles show that in PD, the executive
advanced PD patients but until recently not to cognition,although Marie et al. reported an inverse correla-tion between putamen [11C] nomifensine uptake and
ÃCorresponding author. Department of Neurology, University Medical
associative learning. Most studies have shown that dimin-
Center Groningen, University of Groningen, P.O. Box 30001, 9700 RB
ished (posterior) putamen FDOPA uptake correlates with
Groningen, The Netherlands. Tel.: +31 503616161; fax: +31 503611707.
clinical severity of PD motor symptomatology.
1353-8020/$ - see front matter r 2007 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:
M. van Beilen et al. / Parkinsonism and Related Disorders 14 (2008) 224–228
This study was performed to determine whether striatal
PD following the criteria of the UK Parkinson’s Disease Society Brain
FDOPA uptake is related to cognitive functioning in a
Bank criteria and showed sustained levodopa responsiveness. They wererecruited from our outpatients’ Movement Disorder Unit between
sample of advanced PD patients. While previous cognition
October 1999 and July 2003. All patients suffered from severe
FDOPA-PET studies with PD patients involved early or
pharmacotherapy resistant tremor or intractable motor fluctuations,
heterogeneous (both early and advanced patients) samples
despite optimal antiparkinsonian treatment (levodopa and dopamine
of patients, our study included only moderately to severely
agonists, n ¼ 28; amantadine, n ¼ 6; anticholinergics, n ¼ 5) and were
advanced PD patients. The nature of these relationships
suitable candidates for bilateral deep brain stimulation (DBS) of thethalamus (thalamus DBS, n ¼ 3) or subthalamic nucleus (STN-DBS,
between striatal dopamine uptake and cognitive function-
n ¼ 25), respectively. On a 1 (elementary school not finished) to 7
ing was studied to find an answer to the following question:
(university degree) Dutch scale for education the patients’ median score
are these deficits mainly related to the dopaminergic
was 4 (S.D. 1.4). All patients gave their informed consent prior to study
function in the caudate, the putamen, or both?
inclusion according to the Declaration of Helsinki.
Preoperatively all patients were clinically and neuropsychologically
evaluated following the Core Assessment Program for Surgical Interven-tional Therapy in Parkinson’s Disease (CAPSIT-PD) . Additional in-
Twenty-eight right-handed PD patients with moderately to severely
and exclusion criteria were: a positive levodopa response (at least 33%
advanced disease (modified Hoehn and Yahr Staging in off-medication
improvement of the motor part (III) of the Unified Parkinson’s Disease
condition: 2.5 (n ¼ 1), 3 (n ¼ 13), 4 (n ¼ 9), 5 (n ¼ 5)) participated in the
Rating Scale (UPDRS III) , no depression (Montgomery and Asberg
study. There were 15 men and 13 women (age 6077.4 years, disease
Depression Rating Scale (MADRS), scoreo19) or recent psychiatric
duration 11.874.5 years) (see also ). All patients had idiopathic
illness, no dementia (Mattis Dementia Rating Scale, score4130), and noabnormalities on cerebral MRI suggestive of atypical parkinsonism.
All clinical, neuropsychological and FDOPA-PET assessments were
Clinical characteristics and striatal FDOPA uptake values of the study
performed on separate successive days during hospitalization for 2–4 days
within 3–6 months prior to planned surgery (bilateral thalamus-DBS orSTN-DBS). Antiparkinsonian medication was kept unchanged during a
period of at least 2 weeks prior to every assessment. The UPDRS part IIIscore was assessed in the off-medication condition (after withholding
regular antiparkinsonian drugs for 12 h overnight) and performed by the
same investigator (ATP). The neuropsychological testing were performed
in the medication-on condition following the recommendations of
CAPSIT-PD. All subjects were scanned in the medication-on condition.
Neuropsychological tests included measures for Memory, Executive
functioning, and Fluency. Memory was tested with the memory scale of
the Matthis Dementia Rating Scale (MDRS) and the 15 Words Test
(15 WT) Learning score (sum score of 15 words that were presented five
times) and Recall score (words remembered after a 20 min delay).
Executive functioning was tested with the Odd Man Out (OMO) test for
cognitive switching, the Stroop Color-Word Card divided by the Stroop
Color Card (Stroop Interference Index), and the time needed on theTrailmaking B divided by the time needed on the Trailmaking A (TRAIL
AB, cognitive switching). Fluency performance was measured with several
tests including two categorical tests (Animals and Professions, 1 min each),
and three letter Fluency tests. As will be seen later, for each concept
(Memory, Executive functioning, and Fluency) aggregate scores across the
All PET measurements were performed at the UMCG PET Center on a
Siemens ECAT Exact HR+ (n ¼ 13) or ECAT 951 (n ¼ 15) scanner. The
two different scanners were optimally calibrated so as to ensure that the
bLEDD: Levodopa Equivalent Daily Dose ¼ levodopa dose (100 mg) Â 1
measurements are comparable for all practical purposes. Subjects were
(added with 0.2 Â levodopa dose if using entacapone with each dose)+(slow
positioned supine in a resting state with their eyes closed and ears
release levodopa  0.7)+bromocriptine  10+ropinirole  20+pergolide Â
unplugged. After pretreatment with 2 mg/kg carbidopa to block peripheral
dopa-decarboxylase activity, 180733 MBq of FDOPA was intravenously
cReference values FDOPA uptake, UMC Groningen: healthy volunteers
injected over 1 min with an infusion pump. All subjects were measured
(n ¼ 10, age 56719 years): 1.6970.29 (putamen), 1.6870.25 (caudate);
following a static or dynamic protocol with identical time range for data
PD (n ¼ 18, age 6476 years, disease duration 973 years): 0.7970.1
analysis. The static protocol consisted of one single scan from 90 to
120 min post-injection, while the dynamic protocol consisted of 21 time
M. van Beilen et al. / Parkinsonism and Related Disorders 14 (2008) 224–228
frames with increasing duration over 120 min: then the last two frames
component of the variables at hand. These turned out to correlate strongly
(2 Â 900 s) were averaged to create a equivalent volume to the static scan.
(over 0.95) with the simple composite scores, and correlations of these
Linear normalization with SPM99 was used to align the measured
with the variables of interest (caudate and putamen FDOPA uptake)
volume data to a rCBF template fixed in Talairach coordinate space .
differed by no more than 0.5 from those based on the simple composite
Region of interest (ROI) analysis was based on a standardized template
scores, and were therefore ignored in the sequel.
fixed in Talairach coordinate space. This template, consisting of 6 ROIs(putamen, caudate and occipital lobe on both sides) was used to sample
the volume data and compute mean ROI activity concentration.
Specific FDOPA uptake was expressed as a striato-occipital ratio
(SOR) index following the equation: SOR index ¼ C
In , we see summaries of various characteristics
(CROI ¼ average (left and right) ROI activity concentration, CREF ¼
of the study group. It can be seen that striatal FDOPA
average occipital activity in the occipital reference region).
uptake values of the study PD group were lower than thePD norm group. Female subjects showed higher putamenFDOPA uptake than males. Significant relationships
between the Disease duration, and Executive functioningwith putamen FDOPA uptake were found (see ),
First three composite scores were computed for the cognitive variables.
This has two reasons, first, neuropsychological tests on their own are not
while disease duration, UPDRS motor speed and the three
always valid and reliable enough; a test relies on multiple cognitive
cognitive functioning composite variables all correlated
functions. In order to measure on cognitive domain level, we need to
significantly with caudate FDOPA uptake. To get further
combine test results. Second, this keeps numbers of variables small.
insight into the shape and strength of the association
Specifically, all original cognitive variables were converted to Z-scoresusing mean values and standard deviations. Composite cognitive scores
between the cognitive variables and the FDOPA uptake
were computed by averaging the Z-scores on the related variables for
variables, scatterplots for these relations are given in .
Memory (MDRS memory scale, 15 WT Learning score, and 15 WT Recallscore), Executive functioning (OMO, Stroop Interference Index, Trail
AB), and Fluency (Animals, Professions, and three letter tests). Theresulting dependence of scores of the observation units is handledadequately in the inferential analysis reported below.
Putamen and caudate FDOPA uptake in our study
The main analyses carried out here consisted of assessing the degree of
group were lower than in our PD norm group, and also
association between mean FDOPA uptake values (combined left and right
within the lower range of those stated in literature ,
putamen; combined left and right caudate) and the composite scores for
probably as a result of more advanced disease status. In
Memory, Executive functioning and Fluency. For these, and other
our study, putaminal FDOPA uptake was not significantly
quantitative variables involved in the analyses carried out here, it wasverified to what extent the sample distributions deviated from normal
related to the clinically evaluated level of motor dysfunc-
distributions, so as to get an impression of possible violations of the
tion (as assessed by the UPDRS part III), although
assumption of normality, by means of visual inspection of QQ plots. The
the correlation was in the expected negative direction.
distribution of the scores on UPDRS, Disease duration, Memory,
Caudate FDOPA uptake was related to motor dysfunction.
Fluency, and Mean FDOPA caudate uptake were nearly normally
A possible explanation for these inconsistent results may be
distributed, while those for LEDD (Levodopa Equivalent Daily Dose),Hoehn and Yahr Staging, Executive functioning, and Mean FDOPA
the fact that UPDRS III scores were measured off-medi-
caudate uptake deviated somewhat more strongly from normality. Even
cation, while FDOPA-PET was measured on-medication.
though due to the small sample sizes, we can infer little on the plausibility
However, this was not tested since this study was aimed at
of population distributions being normal, to be on the safe side, we
the relationships between cognition and FDOPA-PET and
decided to do a nonparametric inferential analysis of all results computed
UPDRS on-medication scores were not measured.
here. Specifically, to give confidence intervals for all reported correlations,we used the bootstrap procedure (e.g., see to compute the so-called
Caudate FDOPA uptake was positively related to
95% percentile intervals, which are known to be good approximations of
cognition. These relationships were expected, based on
95% confidence intervals for correlations. A further advantage of using
(a) previous PD literature on caudate dopaminergic
the bootstrap procedure, is that it is not adversely affected by the
function and cognition; (b) on the findings in other patient
dependence between scores of different observation units as far as this was
groups such as patients with Huntington’s disease ,
created by our procedure of forming composite scores. This is because thebootstrap procedure used here relies on repeatedly (1000 times) resampling
caudate hemorrhage , and (c) on the findings in healthy
observation units with scores on all original variables involved, and then
subjects In PD, caudate dopaminergic function has
doing all computations for the resulting bootstrap sample in the same wayas done for the original sample. Thus, construction of composites was
completely mimicked in the bootstrap analysis. An important property of
Correlation coefficients between disease duration, motor score, and
the bootstrap procedure is that it gives good confidence intervals for
values computed by whatever statistical analysis procedure used, providedthat this is mimicked exactly for each individual bootstrap sample. In this
way, for each correlation, a 95% confidence interval was reported, soas to indicate its accuracy as an estimate of the correlation in the
population. This interval can also be used for significance testing by
verifying whether the null hypothesis value (e.g. a correlation of 0) is in the
interval (not significant for a two-sided test at 0.05 level) or outside
For verification purposes, in addition to the simple composite scores as
computed above, we used composite scores computed as the first principal
M. van Beilen et al. / Parkinsonism and Related Disorders 14 (2008) 224–228
Fig. 1. Scatterplots of Pearson’s R in striatal FDOPA uptake and cognition. PUTM: mean putamen FDOPA uptake; CAUM: mean caudate FDOPAuptake; Exe: composite score for executive functions; Memory: composite score for memory; Fluency: composite score for fluency.
been linked to tests measuring Executive function
and organization of verbal material. However, different
and Memory In particular the interference effect on
aspects of Executive functioning can be distinguished from
the Stroop test has been associated with caudate dopami-
each other; our variable ‘Executive functioning’ concerned
nergic function in addition to measurement of
mostly mental flexibility, while the others concerned the
cognitive switching that included reward for the right
organization of behavior. Mental flexibility includes cogni-
response . However, measurements of cognitive switch-
tive switching and inhibition, while by organization we
ing that did not include reward (i.e. WCST) have not
mean the (semantic) clustering of information (words) we
revealed significant relationships with caudate FDOPA
apply as a retrieval strategy in both verbal memory tasks
uptake in PD . Similarly, Memory has not been
and fluency tests. Putamen FDOPA uptake was related to
consistently related to caudate FDOPA uptake or
the ‘flexibility’ factor in Executive functioning. Monchi
may only show this relationship in more advanced patients
et al. offer a possible explanation for this finding with their
Rinne et al. reported Memory to be related to frontal
activation study in which they investigated brain activation
FDOPA uptake but not to caudate FDOPA uptake .
patterns during neuropsychological testing instead of
Fluency performance was studied in two larger FDOPA-
relying on correlational analyses . They concluded that
PET studies with interesting results: Broussolle et al.
the putamen was activated during actions that followed a
concluded that Fluency is independent from striatal
cognitive switch, i.e. actions according to specific beha-
FDOPA uptake, and Rinne et al. found Fluency to be
vioral rules. The mental components of this cognitive
related to frontal but not to striatal uptake .
switch were unrelated to putamen activity. Since most
Our main finding compared to previous studies is the
neuropsychological tests require such a motor response
association found between putamen FDOPA uptake and
after mental switching, it may be this motor component of
Executive functioning. This is a surprising finding, since
test performance that is related to putamen FDOPA
previous reports and neuroanatomical findings suggest that
uptake. Indeed, our tests (e.g. Trail Making Test and
the putamen is involved in motor functioning but not in
OMO Test) and those of Lozza et al. required motor
cognition. However, other authors have also suggested an
actions after cognitive switching while some of the studies
association between FDOPA uptake in the putamen and
that did not find a relation between putamen FDOPA
cognitive processing using comparable paradigms
uptake and neuropsychological test behavior did not
(FDG-PET and [123I]b-CIT SPECT, respectively), in
include tests that require motor actions after cognitive
addition to findings with other paradigms
switching . However, results are not consistent, some
In our patients, putamen FDOPA uptake was related to
authors did include such a test but did not find putaminal
Executive functioning and Fluency, but not to Memory. It
should be noted that, as addressed in the introduction, the
relationship between putamen and cognition found in the
measures for Memory and Fluency also concern Executive
study of Mu¨ller et al. did involve tests with an action
functioning since they reflect cognitive searching strategies
component, but not specifically an action after switching.
M. van Beilen et al. / Parkinsonism and Related Disorders 14 (2008) 224–228
The methodological aspects of the study should be
[9] Defer GL, Widner H, Marie RM, et al. Core assessment program for
considered in the interpretation of the study results. The
surgical interventional therapies in Parkinson’s disease (CAPSIT-
FDOPA uptake in the striatum was lower in our patient
[10] Fahn S, Elton RL. Members of the UPDRS development committee.
study group as compared to previous studies with de novo
Unified Parkinson’s disease rating scale. In: Fahn S, Marsden CD,
patients. However, it should be noted, that although the
Calne DB, Goldstein M, editors. Recent developments in Parkinson’s
mean striatal FDOPA uptake was low, the range of values
was sufficient for a correlation analysis. Finally, our
[11] Friston KJ. Commentary and opinion. 2. Statistical Parametric
analyses were not corrected for multiple comparisons.
Mapping—ontology and current issues. J Cer Blood Flow Met 1995;15:361–70.
Although most of the correlations would not have
[12] Talairach J, Tournoux P. Co-planar stereotactic atlas of the human
remained significant after a conservative correction for
brain. New York: Thieme Medical Publishers; 1998.
multiple comparisons, the present results, in the light of
[13] Dhawan V, Ma Y, Pillai V, Spetsieris P, Chaly T, Belakhlef A, et al.
previous literature, confirm the a priori hypothesis of
Comparative analysis of striatal FDOPA uptake in Parkinson’s
positive correlations between FDOPA uptake and cogni-
disease: ratio method versus graphical approach. J Nucl Med 2002;43:1324–30.
[14] Efron B, Tibshirani R. Emperical bayes methods and false discovery
In conclusion, putamen FDOPA uptake was related to
rates for microarrays. Genet Epidemiol 2002;23:70–86.
mental flexibility (i.e. cognitive switching) in advanced non-
[15] Ba¨ckman L, Robins-Wahlin TB, Lundin A, et al. Cognitive deficits in
demented PD patients. This may represent difficulty
Huntington’s disease are predicted by dopaminergic PET markers
performing the actions after cognitive switching during
and brain volumes. Brain 1997;20:2207–17.
[16] Fuh HJ, Wang SJ. Caudate hemorrage: clinical features, neuropsy-
chological assessments and radiological findings. Clin Neurol
The caudate may be more important in the mental
components Executive functioning, while the putamen may
[17] Postle BR, D’Esposito M. Dissociation of human caudate nucleus
be more important in the motor components of Executive
activity in spatial and nonspatial working memory: an event-related
fMRI study. Brain Res 1999;16:107–15.
[18] Marie´ RM, Barre´ L, Dupuy B, Viader F, Defer G, Baron JC.
Relationships between striatal dopamine denervation and frontal
executive tests in Parkinson’s disease. Neurosci Lett 1998;260:77–80.
[19] Bru¨ck A, Portin R, Lindell A, Laihinen A, Bergman J, Haaparata M,
This study was sponsored by the Stichting International
et al. Positron emission tomography shows that impaired frontal lobe
Parkinson Fonds (Hoofddorp, The Netherlands).
functioning in Parkinson’s disease is related to dopaminergichypofunction in the caudate nucleus. Neurosci Lett 2002;311:81–4.
[20] Rinne JO, Portin R, Ruottinen H, et al. Cognitive impairment and
the brain dopaminergic system in Parkinson disease [18F]Fluorodopapositron emission tomographic study. Arch Neurol 2000;57:470–5.
[1] Lang AE, Lozano AM. Parkinson’s disease. First of two parts.
[21] Holthoff-Detto VA, Kessler J, Herholz K, Bo¨nner H, Pietrzyk U,
Wu¨rker M, et al. Funcional effects of striatal dysfunction in
[2] Schapira AHV. Parkinson’s disease. Science, medicine, and the
Parkinson disease. Arch Neurol 1997;54:145–50.
[22] Broussolle E, Dentresangle C, Landais P. The relation of putamen
[3] Dubois B, Pillon B. Cognitive deficits in Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol
and caudate nucleus [18F]F-dopa uptake to motor and cognitive
performances in Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol Sci 1999;166:141–51.
[4] Green J, McDonald WM, Vitek JL, Evatt M, Freeman A, Haber M,
[23] Lozza C, Baron JC, Eidelberg D, Mentis MJ, Carbon M, Marie.
et al. Cognitive impairments in advanced PD without dementia.
Executive processes in Parkinson’s diseaese: FDG-PET and network
analysis. Hum Brain Mapping 2004;22:236–45.
[5] Alexander GE, DeLong MR, Strick PL. Parallel organization of
[24] Mu¨ller U, Wa¨chter T, Barthel H, Reuter M, von Cramon DY.
functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex.
Striatal [123I]b-CIT SPECT and prefrontal cognitive functions in
Parkinson’s disease. J Neural Transmission 2000;107:303–19.
[6] Leenders KL, Salmon EP, Tyrell Perani P, Brooks DJ, Sager H,
[25] Karnath H, Himmelbach M, Rorden C. The subcortical anatomy of
Jones H, et al. The nigrostriatal system assessed in vivo by positron
human spatial neglect: putamen, caudate nucleus and pulvinar. Brain
emission tomography in healthy volunteers and patients with
Parkinson’s disease. Arch Neurol 1990;47:1290–8.
[26] Troyer AK, Black SE, Armilio ML, Moscovitch M. Cognitive and
[7] Antonini A, Vontobel P, Psylla M, Gunther I, Maguire PR, Missimer
motor functioning in a patient with selective infarction of the left
J, et al. Complementary positron emission tomographic studies of the
basal ganglia: evidence for decreased non-routine response selection
striatal dopaminergic system in Parkinson’s disease. Arch Neurol
and performance. Neuropsychologia 2003;42:902–11.
[27] Monchi O, Petrides M, Petre V, Worsley K, Dagher A. Wisconsin
[8] Marie´ RM, Barre´ L, Dupuy B, Viader F, Defer G, Baron JC.
card sorting revisited: distinct neural circuits participating in different
Relationships between striatal dopamine denervation and frontal
stages of the task identified by event-related functional magnetic
executive tests in Parkinson’s disease. Neurosci Lett 1998;260:77–80.
resonance imaging. J Neurosci 2001;21:7733–41.
N O V E M B E R 2 0 0 3 Members of the Dermatological Drugs in Pregnancy Division of Dermatology By MOHAMMED AL-HADDAB, MD, DENIS SASSEVILLE, MD, FRCPC Three percent of all children born in the United States have a major structural malforma- tion that is detectable at birth1 and at least 10% of birth defects are thought to result from maternal drug exposure.2 The approach of the
INSTRUCTIONS FOR PATIENTS FOLLOWING SURGERY T (780) 482-6551 YOU ARE LEGALLY IMPAIRED FOR 24 HOURS FOLLOWING A GENERAL ANESTHETIC. DO NOT DRIVE OR OPERATE HAZARDOUS MACHINERY. T (780) 482-6551 T (780) 482-6551 1. Bleeding Some bleeding is normal following your surgery. To control bleeding, place a tightly rolled gauze over the surgical site and apply firm, constant bi